Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 9 de 9
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 1390, 2022 01 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35082319

RESUMO

Pupillometry has become a standard measure for assessing arousal state. However, environmental factors such as luminance, a primary dictator of pupillary responses, often vary across studies. To what degree does luminance interact with arousal-driven pupillary changes? Here, we parametrically assessed luminance-driven pupillary responses across a wide-range of luminances, while concurrently manipulating cognitive arousal using auditory math problems of varying difficulty. At the group-level, our results revealed that the modulatory effect of cognitive arousal on pupil size interacts multiplicatively with luminance, with the largest effects occurring at low and mid-luminances. However, at the level of individuals, there were qualitatively distinct individual differences in the modulatory effect of cognitive arousal on luminance-driven pupillary responses. Our findings suggest that pupillometry as a measure for assessing arousal requires more careful consideration: there are ranges of luminance levels that are more ideal in observing pupillary differences between arousal conditions than others.


Assuntos
Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Nível de Alerta/efeitos da radiação , Luz , Pupila/fisiologia , Pupila/efeitos da radiação , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/efeitos da radiação , Estimulação Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Cognição/fisiologia , Cognição/efeitos da radiação , Feminino , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular/efeitos da radiação , Frequência Cardíaca/fisiologia , Frequência Cardíaca/efeitos da radiação , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Tela , Adulto Jovem
2.
J. optom. (Internet) ; 10(3): 176-188, jul.-sept. 2017. graf, tab
Artigo em Inglês | IBECS | ID: ibc-164147

RESUMO

Purpose: Letter sequence recognition accuracy has been postulated to be limited primarily by low-level visual factors. The influence of high level factors such as visual memory (load and decay) has been largely overlooked. This study provides insight into the role of these factors by investigating the interaction between letter sequence recognition accuracy, letter string length and report condition. Methods: Letter sequence recognition accuracy for trigrams and pentagrams were measured in 10 adult subjects for two report conditions. In the complete report condition subjects reported all 3 or all 5 letters comprising trigrams and pentagrams, respectively. In the partial report condition, subjects reported only a single letter in the trigram or pentagram. Letters were presented for 100 ms and rendered in high contrast, using black lowercase Courier font that subtended 0.4◦ at the fixation distance of 0.57 m. Results: Letter sequence recognition accuracy was consistently higher for trigrams compared to pentagrams especially for letter positions away from fixation. While partial report increased recognition accuracy in both string length conditions, the effect was larger for pentagrams, and most evident for the final letter positions within trigrams and pentagrams. The effect of partial report on recognition accuracy for the final letter positions increased as eccentricity increased away from fixation, and was independent of the inner/outer position of a letter. Conclusions: Higher-level visual memory functions (memory load and decay) play a role in letter sequence recognition accuracy. There is also suggestion of additional delays imposed on memory encoding by crowded letter elements (AU)


Objetivo: Se ha postulado que la precisión del reconocimiento de la secuencia de letras se ve limitada por los factores visuales de bajo nivel. La influencia de los factores de alto nivel, tales como la memoria visual (carga y deterioro) se ha ignorado en muchas ocasiones. Este estudio aporta mayor información sobre la función de dichos factores, al investigar la interacción entre la precisión del reconocimiento de la secuencia de letras, la longitud de la cadena de letras, y la condición reportada. Métodos: Se midió la precisión del reconocimiento de la secuencia de letras para trigramas y pentagramas en 10 sujetos adultos, para dos condiciones de reporte. En la condición de reporte completa, los sujetos reportaron las 3 ó 5 letras incluidas en los trigramas y pentagramas, respectivamente. En la condición de reporte parcial, los sujetos reportaron únicamente una letra del trigrama o pentagrama. Las letras se presentaron durante 100 milisegundos en alto contraste, con fuente y letra minúscula Courier, subtendiendo 0,4 grados a una distancia de fijación de 0,57 m. Resultados: La precisión del reconocimiento de la secuencia de letras fue consistentemente superior en los trigramas, en comparación a los pentagramas, y en especial para las posiciones de las letras alejadas de la fijación. A pesar de que el reporte parcial incrementó la precisión del reconocimiento en ambas situaciones de longitud de la cadena, el efecto fue superior en los pentagramas, y más evidente para las posiciones de la letra final de los trigramas y pentagramas. El efecto del reporte parcial en la precisión del reconocimiento para las posiciones de la letra final se incrementó a medida que se incrementó la excentricidad alejándose de la fijación, siendo independiente de la posición interna/externa de una letra. Conclusiones: Las funciones de la memoria visual de mayor nivel (carga y deterioro de memoria) juegan una función en la precisión del reconocimiento de la secuencia de letras. Esto sugiere también unas demoras adicionales impuestas sobre la codificación de la memoria, por parte de los elementos del amontonamiento de letras (AU)


Assuntos
Humanos , Adulto , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Fisiológico de Modelo/fisiologia , Optometria/métodos , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular/efeitos da radiação , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Leitura , Reconhecimento Fisiológico de Modelo/efeitos da radiação , Psicofísica/métodos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/efeitos da radiação
3.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 77(6): 2153-65, 2015 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25962456

RESUMO

The gap effect refers to a reduction in saccadic reaction time (SRT) to an eccentric target, when the fixation point is removed before the target onset. Though it is known that the gap effect peaks when the fixation point is offset about 200 ms before the onset of the eccentric target, it is unknown how this effect is modulated by stimulus variations. In this paper, we propose and investigate a model of saccadic reaction time as a function of the fixation point brightness gain. The brightness gain is defined as the ratio of the final and initial intensities of the stimulus. We have conducted a typical gap effect experiment with 15 participants, where the brightness of the fixation point was manipulated under four conditions and two gap intervals, at the same time and 200 ms before the onset of the eccentric target. The conditions included removing the fixation point (offset), leaving it with constant brightness (overlap), reducing, and increasing its brightness (lower and higher brightness conditions). Experimental data showed a significant gap effect in the offset and lower brightness conditions when compared to the overlap condition. On the other hand, the SRT was significantly longer for the higher brightness condition than the SRT for the overlap condition. Linear regression analysis using ten values of brightness gain shows that our model fits the data well for the 0- and 200-ms gap, with a coefficient of determination of .89 and .94, respectively.


Assuntos
Fixação Ocular/efeitos da radiação , Raio , Modelos Psicológicos , Tempo de Reação/efeitos da radiação , Movimentos Sacádicos/efeitos da radiação , Percepção Visual/efeitos da radiação , Adulto , Atenção/efeitos da radiação , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estimulação Luminosa , Adulto Jovem
4.
Exp Brain Res ; 233(6): 1893-905, 2015 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25832623

RESUMO

When we explore the visual environment around us, we produce sequences of very precise eye movements aligning the objects of interest with the most sensitive part of the retina for detailed visual processing. A copy of the impending motor command, the corollary discharge, is sent as soon as the first saccade in a sequence is ready to monitor the next fixation location and correctly plan the subsequent eye movement. Neurophysiological investigations have shown that chemical interference with the corollary discharge generates a distinct pattern of spatial errors on sequential eye movements, with similar results also from clinical and TMS studies. Here, we used saccadic inhibition to interfere with the temporal domain of the first of two subsequent saccades during a standard double-step paradigm. In two experiments, we report that the temporal interference on the primary saccade led to a specific error in the final landing position of the second saccade that was consistent with previous lesion and neurophysiological studies, but without affecting the spatial characteristics of the first eye movement. On the other hand, single-step saccades were differently influence by the flash, with a general undershoot, more pronounced for larger saccadic amplitude. These findings show that a flashed visual transient can disrupt saccadic updating in a double-step task, possibly due to the mismatch between the planned and the executed saccadic eye movement.


Assuntos
Fixação Ocular/efeitos da radiação , Transtornos da Motilidade Ocular/fisiopatologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Inibição Psicológica , Masculino , Orientação/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação , Sístole , Adulto Jovem
5.
PLoS One ; 8(5): e63229, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23658814

RESUMO

To investigate refractive and axial responses to the shift of focal plane resulting from the interchange of two monochromatic lights separately corresponding to the peak wavelengths of the cones absorption spectrum in retina, fifty 2-week-old pigmented guinea pigs were randomly assigned to five groups based on the mode of illumination: short-wavelength light (SL), middle-wavelength light (ML) and broad-band white light (BL) for 20 weeks, SL for 10 weeks followed by ML for 10 weeks (STM), as well as ML for 10 weeks followed by SL for 10 weeks (MTS). Biometric and refractive measurements were then performed every 2 weeks. After 10 weeks, SL and STM groups became more hyperopic and had less vitreous elongation than BL group. However, ML and MTS groups became more myopic and had more vitreous elongation. After interchange of the monochromatic light, the refractive error decreased rapidly by about 1.93D and the vitreous length increased by 0.14 mm in STM group from 10 to 12 weeks. After that, there were no significant intergroup differences between STM and BL groups. The interchange from ML to SL quickly increased the refractive error by about 1.53D and decreased the vitreous length by about 0.13 mm in MTS group after two weeks. At this time, there were also no significant intergroup differences between MTS and BL groups. The guinea pig eye can accurately detect the shift in focal plane caused by interchange of two monochromatic lights and rapidly generate refractive and axial responses. However, an excessive compensation was induced. Some properties of photoreceptors or retina may be changed by the monochromatic light to influence the following refractive development.


Assuntos
Fixação Ocular/efeitos da radiação , Luz , Refração Ocular/efeitos da radiação , Animais , Cor , Cobaias , Erros de Refração/fisiopatologia , Corpo Vítreo/fisiologia , Corpo Vítreo/fisiopatologia , Corpo Vítreo/efeitos da radiação
6.
J Neurosci ; 28(27): 6938-49, 2008 Jul 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18596168

RESUMO

The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) plays a role in spatial updating of goals for eye and arm movements across saccades, but less is known about its role in updating perceptual memory. We reported previously that transsaccadic memory has a capacity for storing the orientations of three to four Gabor patches either within a single fixation (fixation task) or between separate fixations (saccade task). Here, we tested the role of the PPC in transsaccadic memory in eight subjects by simultaneously applying single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the right and left PPC, over several control sites, and comparing these to behavioral controls with no TMS. In TMS trials, we randomly delivered pulses at one of three different time intervals around the time of the saccade, or at an equivalent time in the fixation task. Controls confirmed that subjects could normally retain at least three visual features. TMS over the left PPC and a control site had no significant effect on this performance. However, TMS over the right PPC disrupted memory performance in both tasks. This TMS-induced effect was most disruptive in the saccade task, in particular when stimulation coincided more closely with saccade timing. Here, the capacity to compare presaccadic and postsaccadic features was reduced to one object, as expected if the spatial aspect of memory was disrupted. This finding suggests that right PPC plays a role in the spatial processing involved in transsaccadic memory of visual features. We propose that this process uses saccade-related feedback signals similar to those observed in spatial updating.


Assuntos
Transtornos da Memória/fisiopatologia , Memória/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana/métodos , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Campos Eletromagnéticos/efeitos adversos , Retroalimentação/fisiologia , Retroalimentação/efeitos da radiação , Feminino , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular/efeitos da radiação , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Memória/efeitos da radiação , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Lobo Parietal/efeitos da radiação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/efeitos da radiação , Estimulação Luminosa , Movimentos Sacádicos/efeitos da radiação , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/efeitos da radiação , Fatores de Tempo , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana/efeitos adversos
7.
Health Phys ; 85(5): 567-77, 2003 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14571989

RESUMO

During long-term viewing of a continuous light source, head and eye movements affect the distribution of energy deposited in the retina. Previous studies of eye movements during a fixation task provided data used for revising the safety limits for long-term viewing of such sources. These studies have been continued to determine the effect of source brightness on the nature of fixational eye movements. Volunteers fixated for 50 s on a HeNe laser (lambda = 632.8 nm) masked by a small aperture to produce a target subtending approximately 0.03 mrad in the visual field. The source was attenuated to yield corneal irradiance values in the range 0.6 pW cm(-2) to 6 microW cm(-2). Eye movements were recorded using a Dual Purkinje Image Eyetracker. The data were characterized by fixation ellipses that represent areas of the retina in which the image of the spot was located 68% of the time of each trial. Significant variation across subjects in the tightness of fixation was observed. Over the eight orders of magnitude of source brightness used in this experiment (10(-13) to 10(-6) W cm(-2)), no subject showed more than roughly a factor of two variation in the area of the fixation ellipse. No statistically significant trend in tightness of fixation as a function of source brightness was observed. There was no loss of ability to fixate, nor any drive to aversion, at the higher source intensities.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/efeitos da radiação , Lasers , Proteção Radiológica/métodos , Radiometria/métodos , Retina/fisiologia , Retina/efeitos da radiação , Adulto , Idoso , Segurança de Equipamentos/métodos , Segurança de Equipamentos/normas , Feminino , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular/efeitos da radiação , Humanos , Masculino , Doses de Radiação , Proteção Radiológica/normas , Radiometria/normas , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Sensibilidade e Especificidade
8.
Eur J Neurosci ; 17(4): 870-8, 2003 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12603277

RESUMO

Experiments were performed to assess the excitability of neural elements activated while inducing saccadic eye movements electrically from different cortical layers of striate cortex (area V1) in rhesus monkeys. Excitability was assessed by measuring current thresholds, saccadic latencies, chronaxies, and the effectiveness of anode-first vs. cathode-first pulses. Minimum current thresholds for the evocation of saccades (i.e. less than 5 microA) were observed when the deepest layers of V1 were stimulated. The shortest saccadic latencies were also observed at these depths. The shortest latency at 10 times the threshold current was 49 ms on average. The chronaxies of the elements mediating saccades were less in deep V1 (i.e. 0.17 ms) than in superficial V1 (i.e. 0.23 ms). Anode-first pulses were more effective at evoking saccades from superficial V1, whereas cathode-first pulses were more effective at evoking saccades from deep V1. These results indicate that the excitability properties of superficial and deep V1 are distinct for the generation of saccades. Moreover, the excitability of elements mediating saccades in V1 of monkeys is comparable to that of elements mediating phosphenes in human V1.


Assuntos
Estimulação Elétrica/métodos , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/efeitos da radiação , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/efeitos da radiação , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Cronaxia/efeitos da radiação , Relação Dose-Resposta à Radiação , Eletrodos , Fixação Ocular/efeitos da radiação , Macaca mulatta , Probabilidade , Tempo de Reação/efeitos da radiação , Reforço Psicológico , Limiar Sensorial/efeitos da radiação , Córtex Visual/anatomia & histologia , Campos Visuais
9.
J Neurophysiol ; 79(5): 2766-81, 1998 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9582243

RESUMO

Visual fixation, the act of maintaining the eyes directed toward a location of interest, is a highly skilled behavior necessary for high-level vision in primates. In spite of its significance, visual fixation is not well understood; it is not even clear what attributes of the visual input are used to control fixation. Here we show, in four Macaca fascicularis monkeys, that the position the eyes assume during fixation depends on the luminance of the background. Dark background yields fixation positions that are shifted upward with respect to the fixation positions obtained with a dimly illuminated, featureless background. This phenomenon was observed previously in a nutshell by Snodderly; here first we rigorously establish its existence by testing proper controls. We then study the properties of this upshift of the fixation position. We show that, although the size of the upshift varies between monkeys, for all monkeys the upshift is larger than the radius of the fovea. Hence, if the background is dim, the eyes are positioned during fixation so that the target does not fall on the fovea. The size of the upshift remains almost unchanged while the eyes fixate at different orbital positions; thus the upshift is not caused by orbital mechanics. The upshift clearly is present even at the first days of training, but with additional training in fixation with dark background, the upshift increases in size. The upshift rotates with the head. The upshift increases gradually with decreasing levels of background luminosity. Luminosity, not visual contrast, is indeed the primary variable determining the size of the upshift. The contribution of a unit area of the retina to the upshift decreases as inverse square root of distance from the target; therefore, it is the perifoveal region of the retina that mostly contributes to the upshift, while the far periphery has little influence. The upshift can be induced or be canceled in the midst of a fixation by changing the background illumination; hence, the upshift is indeed an attribute of the fixation control system. Finally, the fixation-upshift studied here is different from a previously reported upshift of the endpoints of memory-guided saccades with respect to their target locations. These two types of upshift add up to each other. In discussing the function of the upshift, we note a possible morphological analogue with the retinal rod distribution. The upshift moves the line of gaze to a point intermediate between the fovea and the "dorsal rod peak." The upshift thus may improve visual acuity in scotopic conditions. The brain structure in which the upshift is generated must be involved in both ocular control and visual sensation. We consider several possibilities, of which we regard as the most likely the cerebellum and superior colliculus.


Assuntos
Fixação Ocular/efeitos da radiação , Iluminação , Animais , Sensibilidades de Contraste , Escuridão , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Cabeça/fisiologia , Macaca fascicularis , Postura
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...